1995-04-02: Making Connections: New Understanding of Consciousness
Audio
Audio file
Content type |
Content type
|
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Collection(s) |
Collection(s)
|
||||
Title |
Title
Title
1995-04-02: Making Connections: New Understanding of Consciousness
|
||||
Resource Type |
Resource Type
|
||||
Description |
Description
Vol. III, No. XIV
Transcription: Pribram, (RU) Tease With Good Reason Volume II, XIV "Making Connections: New Understandings of Consciousness" The quest to unlock the workings of the human mind continue to intrigue us. Next on With Good Reason, an interview with world-renowned brain scientist Karl Pribram. I'm Carolyn Elliott. Music Up Is the music too loud? If your answer is yes, chances are you're a woman. It's sexism with a scientific basis. New findings suggest the female brain is more sensitive to loud sounds than the male brain. Thus men like their music a few decibels higher than women. But on the other hand -- if the lighting in a room seems too dim, the opposite is true. The male brain sees more light than its female counterpart. An explanation for why women turn lights on and men turn them off. These are just a few recent breakthroughs in brain science. Others are breaking almost daily. Appropriate for the 1990's, which President Clinton has declared, "The Decade of the Brain". My guest today is a pioneer in developing a revolutionary idea of how the brain works ... one that may shed light on phenomena like emotion, desire and the human need to understand the world and ourselves. Dr. Karl Pribram's holographic theory of the brain was received with skepticism when he first proposed it in the 1970's. But new advances in computer modeling and lab work are slowly proving him to be right. Dr. Karl Pribram is an eminent professor of psychology at Radford University and joins me today. INTERVIEW Elliott Dr. Pribram, most of us have a general idea of how the nervous system works. Touch your finger to a hot stove and a message fires back to take your finger off the stove. But the brain doesn't work on such a simple stimulus-response fashion-) Can you give us an idea of how the brain works, a general idea of what your holonomic or holographic theory is? Pribram (yes, let's go back to the reflex, because it's simple, it's 1 easy. And what you described is correct, but not quite. Starting in about the 1950's, we began to see that these so-called "reflexes" operate much more the way thermostats do. Sure, a thermostat turns on when the room gets cold, turns the furnace on, or the air-conditioning on if it's too hot. But you can control it. 7 It's like a bunch of thermostats that can be controlled,-but some houses for instance have thermostats in the living room and another one in the bedroom and they work in parallel. The actual circuitry has in it this parallel distributor processing level which is like thermostats where settings are made constantly of different parts of the circuits, frontal, middle, back part. Hundreds of different circuits are constantly being reset on the basis of what else is going on in the brain. As for instance right now, a lot of our circuits are being set to concentrate on each other and ignore any possible distractions that might be happening in our surrounds. Elliott And I'm trying to think of what question I'm going to ask next. Pribram Exactly, and at the same time, and realize that when you talk to me as I'm talking to you, we're not really aware or conscious of the words that are corning out of our mouth. You're already planning what you are going to ask me next, and I'm already planning in my brain, I'm changing the settings in my brain. And we have, when I was at Stanford we already did experiments showing the kind of process you mentioned, the holonomic theory. The only kind of process that we know that would be reactive enough and have enough memory capability is the kind of process that we talked about in the synapsis, in those connections between circuits so that, and we showed actually that this works, that this mechanism really works in that fashion, in the motor systems. Elliott Well we came up with, the staff of With Good Reason, was brainstorming before this interview, trying to think of some questions we could ask you about the way that our mind works, and thought that perhaps you could come up with some explanations. One thing that had happened to all of us, and happens to most people, is you work very hard and you're trying to figure out a puzzle or a problem, spend hours on it and your head hurts, and all of a sudden that light bulb goes off, all of a sudden you understand. What's going on in the brain that all of a sudden the light bulb pops on and it's all crystal clear. Pribram If you'll interview me in five or ten years I hope to have 2 the answer for you. That's the kind of question, that question has been asked now for 150 years and Harvard here took up the problem, "What is the stream of thought". What's going on that suddenly you get an answer, and what they found originally is that no two people do it the same way. Some people work at it, as you described, other people go to sleep, and wake up with the answer. Still other people have the answer come to them just at that juncture between sleep and wakefulness. And some people write all the answers down. Elliott But could it be all the parts of the thermostat ... Pribram ... coming together. That's right. What Freud said, if you just talk in a fairly free situation but with the answer of what you're looking for, at least in mind, you know of what the answer might, where you're directing it, it was called free-association, it isn't, it was, you've got a problem and you want to ... Elliott So you're just kind of doing it verbally. Pribram Right, and so the whole Freudian endeavor really is, though most people don't know this, really aims at the question that you've just asked. Elliott States of intense concentration. We've all been in a situation like working on a deadline where the brain seems to shut out everything. You don't get hungry. You don't feel sleepy. A bomb could go off and you couldn't hear it because you're just focused on getting that one thing done. What's happening in your mind, and the mental processes to allow you just to blank everything out? Pribram Well since I'm a brain scientist I don't know what's going on in your mind but I do hope to know what's going on in the brain when that happens. It looks as if the frontal lobe, the very far front of the brain, becomes involved in order to allow you to concentrate on something very tightly. And if you do that, that shuts out distractions and the indirect evidence is that if we electricly stimulate this part of the brain in monkeys, then we change the input channels to the senses. Elliott So if I see you the way I see you know and then you give me this electrical stimulation, would I be seeing you differently? Pribram 3 Correct. Pribram You've probably had this experience if you've been in love and had somebody touch you. It feels different. Elliott Now you're embarrassing me. Pribram But it feels different, doesn't it? Elliott Yes. Pribram And if I touch you, or if some bloke, you know, comes in and touches you, it's so different, the feel. Well the same thing goes in "Beauty is in the eye of the beholder", we have various ways of describing this. Elliott And my understanding is that the kind of upshot of all of this is that the frontal lobe is kind of the control center for the brain, and you can have things stored all over the brain, but in order to pull them all together your frontal lobe has got to be functioning correctly. Pribram Certainly. I wouldn't go quite that far but most of the time the brain becomes organized by the input. So most of the time it's the external situation that determines that. But there are ambiguities, there are ambiguous situations where, when you just don't know what. At that point you need an executive, and he just does things on the basis of previous experience. It's not all that difficult. Elliott Oh really? The frontal lobe is organizing on the basis of previous experience? Pribram Sure. last time, five, what or ... ? ". Elliott You know, you remember things better, "It worked that let's try it again. It didn't work three times out of do you do then? Well, should I give it another chance That sounds very computer-like. 4 Pribram Well, the brain is very much like a computer although many scientist and philosophers right now are damning that analogy, the analogy is still a very useful one up to a point. And that point of course is emotions and so on and so forth. Elliott You head up Radford University's Center for Brain Research and Informational Sciences, which the acronym for that is "BRAINS". And your work includes studying pain control, and that brings the frontal lobe back in because you've also shown that the frontal lobe has determination in whether you actually feel pain. Can you tell us a little bit about that ongoing work? Pribram What we do is very simple. We stimulate the middle finger with an electric current which is pulsed, and you can change the strength of that current and when you get up to a certain strength it feels uncomfortable. Then we tell the patient, the subject, under hypnosis to go away and enjoy themselves, go to the beach with their girlfriend or boyfriend or go to a concert and forget all this. When these subjects are at the beach or at a concert hall the frontal lobe seems to have electrical activity which suggests that it's saying to the rest of the brain, "Don't pay attention, don't pay attention, don't pay attention," every time the stimulus goes on. And in other subjects that frontal lobe activity is more pronounced, sort of, at the end of the interval, let's say 500 milliseconds after the stimulus. We don't know what the difference is between these two classes of subjects. Elliott So what do you hypothesize is going on in the mind to make them not feel? Pribram You have to make a conceptual leap here. I've claimed since I wrote a book Languages of the Brain in '71, that what we are aware of is what's going on in the cerebral cortex in the synaptic dendritic level ... Elliott The cerebral cortex being? Pribram The bark. Elliott The bark on the outside of the brain, okay. 5 Pribram And it has layers in it, and it has nerve cells, and the nerve cells have these connection points that you talked about. Where the connections are within the branches of the nerve cells. So, they're called dendrites. That's a Latin word for branching. And so, at this synapto-dendritic level, branching, where all these branches connect up, it's what's going on there that we are aware of. And the longer it's going on there, the more awareness there is. And frontal lobes are there to tune the mechanism of how long all of this is going to last. Now after lobotomy, on days that people used to do them, I took a set of subjects, patients, and they had intractable pain, and I would play chess or checkers with them, depending on whether they could play. And they would keep interrupting, you know, "I can't concentrate now, I've got so much pain," and so on and so forth. After the lobotomy I'd simply time how often they would come in with their, you know they've got pain and it was almost nil. And even if I'd ask them, "How's it feel, how's your pain doing?", they'd say, "Well, it hurts, yeah," and then they'd make the next move. I mean it just shortened the duration. Elliott Is that because the reactions aren't going on or is it because the frontal lobe isn't organizing the reactions for you to sense them? Pribram keep And Well, in normal, normally you use your frontal lobe to paying attention as you said, to concentrate on something. once the pain gets going, it's there, and the concentration is there for it, and you can't get rid of it. Elliott I may be making a jump here but does your research suggests that if the pain is regular, if there are regular throbs, that perhaps you can, we are getting to a point where you can learn not to pay attention to that pain? Pribram Oh sure. In fact David Spiegel who worked with me at Stanford and did some experiments, he quite inadvertently found that with chronic pain patients who have cancer, that simply by doing hypnotic like pro-seeself hypnosis that these people really can control their pain, but the thing that he found that's so fascinating is that these people lived twice as long as the control subjects. Elliott Because they're not feeling the pain. Pribram Well, you said because. 6 Elliott Right. I'm not a scientist, I can make those leaps. Pribram Well, one of the things about holonomic brain theory is that the human brain is so organized that it can make correlations so easily. That's how we know this is an entity, cause we just correlate, correlate, correlate. That's how these brain scans are done. That's how they're done. You get a picture from this side, from this side, from this side. Elliott Using an MRI, correct me if I'm wrong, an MRI or something called a PET scan, you can actually watch the processes going on in the brain. Pribram And many of them are parallel. But that just tells you at a very superficial level that many things are going on simultaneously in the brain. The problem with all those techniques is they take so long. And one of the things that we're developing at Radford in the Center for Brain Research is ways of measuring the dynamics of these processes which are going on at a very great rate, in milliseconds. If we can get the newer technologies going and look at the whole brain, not just the top half and you know, and just, and so on. And those techniques are being developed now and we have good reasons .... Elliott So you're talking about really wiring the brain and looking at a computer and seeing, and when I was talking to you before the interview you were referring to that as "firepower" I think. Pribram That's right. I mean 128 electrodes, 128 amplifiers hooked up to 128 channels in a computer that's going to, and then of course the programming of that so that we can learn what's actually going on. But it will be five years, or ten years from now, we'll be able, in much better shape to say, "Yes, hey, you know. It goes like this. Boom-boom-boom!". MUSIC BRIDGE This is With Good Reason, I'm Carolyn Elliott. My guest is Dr. Karl Pribram, who's here to explain why sometimes it does seem like the body doesn't know what the brain is doing. Dr. Pribram spent more than thirty years studying the brain and behavior at Stanford University and today is an eminent professor of 7 psychology at Radford University. He's author of the landmark book Languages of the Brain, which he wrote in 1971. Some of his other books include The Biology of the Mind, The Biology of Emotions and Other Feelings, and Brain. Mind. and Consciousness. Elliott Dr. Pribram, in 1990 you wrote a book on perception, and again it's a complex idea. But I'd like to again list some everyday occurrences that occur with perception and maybe you can help us understand what's happening or what you believe is happening in the brain. Two people go to a museum and they look at a painting and they see completely different things. - Pribram Well, we've already answered that question, haven't we? Because the rest of their brains, especially these higher order locations, so called systems, intrinsic systems I call them, they used to be called association systems, operate top down. And so they alter, they program the way your input is organized. And so the configuration of that input depends on the organization higher up. Elliott And so you're saying no two people's brain have the same program? Pribram Definitely. Elliott But you know, we learned that there are certain things we are supposed to see in a picture. And now you're saying that no two people see it the same way. Pribram Well it depends on the picture. If it's a very simple picture of a triangle, hopefully everybody will see it as a triangle and not as a pentagon. Elliott I'm thinking of more like a Monet or, you know. Pribram Sure, well those you know, you appreciate different aspects, and you yourself will see it differently at different times. Elliott I've saved the most exotic for last. Your theory of the holonomic brain al$o, you have said, may explain spirituality or humanness urges, sensations, love. And you went a step further 8 and said when we understand the spiritual side of humanness, which you believe is connected with understanding the function of the brain, we're going to increase human potential. Pribram As far as the spiritual aspects, the way I define spirit is simply that there seems to be, in humans, the propensity to get in touch with something larger than their individual selves. They can do it in terms of nature, they can do it in terms of religion, they can do it in terms of science. But they always want to know, when did the big bang occur? You know, the cosmology in science, or in a social setting, social science or the fact that we have communal ways of talking about doing things, or religious ways. That's what I mean by spiritual. And of course that organizes the loving aspects, and so the whole body gets into that. What we have suggested is that when the frontal lobes become hyperactive, that is if you really use your frontal lobes as you've said, when you were concentrating. Very often when I do that I feel as though I'm a "through-put". Have you ever, when you're witting, that something is operating through you? Elliott Oh yeah. Pribram It seems from our experiments that when we stimulate the frontal lobes electricly, that what happens at these channels that I was talking about, input channels, sensory input channels, that they, each of them have what's called an inhibitory surround. They are separated from each other. Elliott Each channel separated with something to inhibit them around that. Pribram It isn't interacting. I mean it is an interaction to get the intermission, but they can, in technical terms they can multiplex the whole system so you have, several channels can act independently. It's like having several telephones at home. Now what happens when you stimulate frontal lobes electricly is that all of these run together. The inhibitory surround is gone. The inhibition is inhibited. And so all the channels run together and it's like one big channel. When you do that in a system like a holographic-like system, it looks like a hologram. Like one big area. And so when you do that you have no boundaries. All the boundaries dissolve. Elliott And that's when you get that zooming in, that intense concentration, that kind of ... 9 Pribram Yes. And every one of the spiritual traditions, the practice is always of highly concentrate, and then everything dissolves. Elliott Well that's fascinating. The one thing I will say is that when I get into that mode I do get the distinct feeling that my physical body couldn't take it very long. It's exhausting. Pribram It's when we stimulate the frontal lobe that the organization of the brain is such that we can derive from that, we can infer from that that this must be what's going on. Now we haven't shown it in a particular experiment, that hypnosis stuff that we talked about is the closest to it. But another five years, if everything goes well, I'm really hopeful that we can do this, and really show that when you have a transcendental experience that what is going on in the organization of the entire brain. Elliott Well I wanted to ask you something, because in the past, I'd say five years there's been a real increased interest in spirituality, I mean it's even making it into t.v. with shows about angels. And I'm wondering if that's kind of corresponding with the brain research? As we're being told that there may be explanations for transcendental experiences or spirituality, if people are feeling more free to explore than they were in the past? Pribram I think it is for many people, in my case it's not. As a scientist I've never felt the need to say, "Oh, spiritual experiences don't exist", which so many people feel that if somebody has spiritual experiences or that somebody describes them, "Oh they're nuts!", and we don't want to deal with it. I just feel that's very unscientific. If somebody describes something to me they may be charlatan, they may be putting me on, there's no way I have of knowing "it. I see magicians working and I know they're doing magic, and you know it's sort of fun. But I've seen people like Yuri Geller do things like bending keys and so on, and he may have a little capsule of chemical, but at least when you look at it under laser light it looks different from any bending that I've or anybody else has ever seen. Let's just accept the fact that we don't understand it. Elliott Deja Vu. Any idea what deja vu is, we all have that? Pribram It has to do with familiarity. And we did about forty years 10 of research on this so it has do with familiarity. "Deja" means "I'm familiar with". You know you've been in a place, and you come in and it feels very unfamiliar. That's "Shame Vu". And that's due to a seizure going on in that particular location of the brain. And Deja Vu is the same thing, when a seizure can set in. You walk into a place that you know from your own experience that you've never been in but it "feels". It feels right in fact. And you can do this in an interpersonal experience too. Elliott Right. them but you kind of ... Pribram. You feel like you know the person, you've never met feel like you've met them someplace. They fit some It's again, it's this part of the brain that seems to be involved in that doesn't have to be pathological. And this part of the brain, one more thing about it, it's also this part of the brain that allows you to take a particular episode and integrate it into your personal history, into the narrative consciousness that makes you feel as though your entity and I ... Elliott I want to give you a chance, right now you're organizing the fourth appalachian conference, and tell us real briefly about this. Pribram. The fourth appalachian conference is going to handle, the title of it is "Learning as Self Organization, not Self Gratification". And it comes out of the history of psychology where learning theory in the 1940's, 30's-40's, where everything was drive reduction. The idea was that the only reason you ever learn something is 'cause if you're hungry, you get to eat. My own experience and the experiences of the very good students that I've seen, it all started with that. That they reorganized their lecture notes. Once or twice or three times during the semester. And when they do that they come out with almost perfect grades every time. And those who don't reorganize, in terms of their own brain organization, their own mental organization, they keep floundering around. Elliott And so at the conference, how are you going to look at this? Pribram. Well, uh, in terms of first of all reenforcement theory, a fellow by the name of Colleen has developed mathematical models, I've been following his work for a number of years. We need computer scientist, with parallel distributing processing kinds of things, to come in and take a look at this and see how much they've done and how those things fit together, and then we want 11 some people who have been working on a nervous system, both experimentally and clinicly. They will go out with new ways of looking at things. Elliott So you're coming at it from three different directions? Pribram Three different directions. Elliott The 1990's have been declared by the President as "The Decade of the Brain". Will there be a big breakthrough by the turn of the century? Pribram Oh the breakthroughs are coming right and left if anybody could recognize them. Well we've described one set of experiments here where we can find out electrical activity of the brain in enough detail so that we can look at the dynamics and correlate it with the thinking process, let's say, and personality and how different people think. So I think that's one major thing. The chemistry has been about since the mid-S0's and maybe even mid-70's, a breakthrough, we'll learn so much more about the brain's chemistry. The same chemical does different things in different parts of the brain, completely different things, maybe even opposite things. Elliott There's so much more I'd like to talk to you about, we didn't get to talk about the male and the female brain and artificial intelligence but we're out of time. Karl Pribram, thank you so much for joining me on With Good Reason. Pribram It has been a great pleasure, thank you for all the preparation that you've done and the good work and asking such really good questions. OUTllO My guest has been Dr. Karl Pribram, eminent professor of psychology at Radford University. On our next show we'll take a look at whether the public should take a more active role in science research. This is With Good Reason. The project coordinator is Michael McDowell. John Wilkinson is the assistant producer. Kevin Piccini wrote the theme music. I'm Carolyn Elliott. "What's the Frequency, Kenneth?" by R.E.M. and Laurie Anderson's "Baby Doll" are both available on Warner Brothers Records. 12 This program was produced with the help of WCVE Richmond, thanks also to WVTF Roanoke. With Good Reason is produced for the Virginia Higher Education Broadcasting Consortium by the State Council of Higher Education and public radio stations serving Virginia. The views expressed are not necessarily those of the consortium or this station. To comment on today's program or to receive tapes or transcripts, call toll free, 1-800-245-2434. 13 |
||||
Local Identifier |
Local Identifier
314
314_1
|
||||
Persons |
Persons
Other (oth): Karl Pribram (Radford University)
|
||||
Genre |
Genre
|
||||
Origin Information |
Origin Information
|
||||
Related Item |
Related Item
|
Language |
English
|
---|---|
Name |
Making Connections: New Understanding of Consciousness
|
MIME type |
audio/mpeg
|
Media Use | |
Authored on |
|
Media of |
37835
|
Download
Audio file